Executive Function and Theory of Mind in Children with ADHD: A Systematic Review

dc.contributor.authorPineda-Alhucema, Wilmar
dc.contributor.authorAristizabal, Edith
dc.contributor.authorEscudero-Cabarcas, Johana
dc.contributor.authorAcosta-López, Johan
dc.contributor.authorVélez, Jorge I.
dc.date.accessioned2018-09-27T14:53:41Z
dc.date.available2018-09-27T14:53:41Z
dc.date.issued2018-08
dc.description.abstractIn developmental research, the relationship between Executive Function (EF) and Theory of Mind (ToM) has been extensively assessed, and EF has been considered a condition for ToM. However, few researchers have studied the relationship between EF and ToM in clinical populations, especially that of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by symptoms of inattention and motor hyperactivity/impulsivity, in which EF is largely impaired. Using the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) model, 201 English and Spanish articles evaluating EF and ToM in ADHD were chosen. Fifteen papers met the inclusion criteria and were selected for further analysis. The first study dates from 2001. Most of the studies’ designs are cross-sectional, include mostly male children, have a small sample size, and were conducted in European countries. Unlike tasks assessing EF, tasks assessing ToM were heterogeneous across studies. The EFs most correlated with ToM were inhibitory control, working memory, cognitive flexibility, and attention. Interest in studying the relationship between EF and ToM in ADHD is recent,but increasing based on new findings and tuning of ToM instruments. However, while an association between EF and ToM is indicated in ADHD, the degree of prediction and predictability of one over the other cannot yet be established because of the studies’ heterogeneity.eng
dc.identifier.issn10407308
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/20.500.12442/2299
dc.language.isoengeng
dc.publisherSpringereng
dc.rights.accessrightsinfo:eu-repo/semantics/openAccess
dc.rights.licenseLicencia de Creative Commons Reconocimiento-NoComercial-CompartirIgual 4.0 Internacionalspa
dc.sourceNeuropsychology Revieweng
dc.sourceVol. 28, No. 3 (2018)spa
dc.source.urihttps://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s11065-018-9381-9eng
dc.subjectTheory of mindeng
dc.subjectExecutive functioneng
dc.subjectADHDeng
dc.subjectInhibitory controleng
dc.subjectSocial cognitioneng
dc.titleExecutive Function and Theory of Mind in Children with ADHD: A Systematic Revieweng
dc.typearticleeng
dcterms.referencesAhmed, F. S., & Miller, S. L. (2011). Executive function mechanisms of theory of mind. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(5), 667–678. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010-1087-7.eng
dcterms.referencesAmerican Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-V). Washington, D.C. EE.UU: Author.eng
dcterms.referencesApperly, I. (2011). Mindreaders. The cognitive basis of “Theory of Mind.”. New York: Psychology Press.eng
dcterms.referencesAron, A. R., & Poldrack, R. A. (2005). The cognitive neuroscience of response inhibition: Relevance for genetic research in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 57(11), 1285–1292. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.10.026.eng
dcterms.referencesArrington, C. N., Kulesz, P., Francis, D., Fletcher, J., & Barnes, M. (2014). The contribution of attentional control and working memory to reading comprehension and decoding. Scientific Studies of Reading, 18, 325–346. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2014.902461.eng
dcterms.referencesAsakura, N., & Inui, T. (2016). A Bayesian framework for false belief reasoning in children: A rational integration of theory-theory and simulation theory. Frontiers in Psychology, 7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.02019.eng
dcterms.referencesAustin, G., Groppe, K., & Elsner, B. (2014). The reciprocal relationship between executive function and theory of mind in middle childhood: A 1-year longitudinal perspective. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00655.eng
dcterms.referencesBaddeley, A. (2018). Exploring working memory: Selected works of Alan Baddeley. EE UU: Routledge.eng
dcterms.referencesBaez, S., Pinasco, C., Roca, M., Ferrari, J., Couto, B., García-Cordero, I., Ibañez, A., Cruz, F., Reyes, P., Matallana, D., Manes, F., Cetcovich, M., & &Torralva T. (2017). Brain structural correlates of executive and social cognition profiles in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia and elderly bipolar disorder. Neuropsychologia, 2017. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.02.012.eng
dcterms.referencesBarkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121(1), 65–94. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.65.eng
dcterms.referencesBarkley, R. A. (2012). Executive functions: What they are, how they work, and why they evolved. New York: Guilford Press.eng
dcterms.referencesBarkley, R. A. (2015). Executive functioning and self-regulation viewed as an extended phenotype. In R. Barkley (Ed.), Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder a handbook for diagnosis and treatment (Chapter 16). New York: Guilford Press.eng
dcterms.referencesBaron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind?”. Cognition, 21, 37–46.eng
dcterms.referencesBaron-Cohen, S., Jolliffe, T., Mortimore, C., & Robertson, M. (1997). Another advanced test of theory of mind: Evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 813–822.eng
dcterms.referencesBaron-Cohen, S., O’Riordan, M., Jones, R., Stone, V. E., & Plaisted, K. (1999). A new test of social sensitivity: Detection of faux pas in normal children and children with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 407–418.eng
dcterms.referencesBaron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Scahill, V., Lawson, J., & Spong, A. (2001). Are intuitive physics and intuitive psychology independent? Journal of Developmental and Learning Disorders, 5, 47–78.eng
dcterms.referencesBenarous, X., Guilé, J.-M., Consoli, A., & Cohen, D. (2015). A systematic review of the evidence for impaired cognitive theory of mind in maltreated children. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 6, 108. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2015.00108.eng
dcterms.referencesBenson, J., & Sabbagh, M. A. (2010). Theory of mind and executive functioning. In P. Zelazo, M. Chandler, & E. Crone (Eds.), Developmental social cognitive neuroscience (pp. 63–80). New York-London: Psychology Press.eng
dcterms.referencesBerenguer, C., Roselló, B., Baixauli, I., García, R., Colomer, C., & Miranda, A. (2017). ADHD symptoms and peer problems: Mediation of executive function and theory of mind. Psicothema, 29(4), 514–517. https://doi.org/10.7334/psicothema2016.376.eng
dcterms.referencesBertoux, M., O’Callaghan, C., Dubois, B., & Hornberger, M. (2016). In two minds: Executive functioning versus theory of mind in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia. Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 87(3), 231–234. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp-2015-311643.eng
dcterms.referencesBigorra, A., Garolera, M., Guijarro, S., & Hervás, A. (2016). Impact of working memory training on hot executive functions (decision-making and theory of mind) in children with ADHD: A randomized controlled trial. Neuropsychiatry, 6(5), 251–263. https://doi.org/10.4172/Neuropsychiatry.1000147.eng
dcterms.referencesBora, E., & Berk, M. (2016). Theory of mind in major depressive disorder: A meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 191, 49–55. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2015.11.023.eng
dcterms.referencesBora, E., & Pantelis, C. (2016). Meta-analysis of social cognition in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Comparison with healthy controls and autistic spectrum disorder. Psychological Medicine, 46, 699–716. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291715002573.eng
dcterms.referencesBora, E., Sehitoglu, G., Aslier, M., Atabay, I., & Veznedaroglu, B. (2007). Theory of mind and unawareness of illness in schizophrenia: Is poor insight a mentalizing deficit? European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 257, 104–111. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-006-0681-3.eng
dcterms.referencesBosco, F. M., Colle, L., De Fazio, S., Bono, A., Ruberti, S., & Tirassa, M. (2009). Th.o.m.a.s.: An exploratory assessment of theory of mind in schizophrenic subjects. Consciousness and Cognition, 18, 306–319. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2008.06.006.eng
dcterms.referencesBosco, F. M., Gabbatore, I., Tirassa, M., & Testa, S. (2016). Psychometric properties of the theory of mind assessment scale in a sample of adolescents and adults. Frontiers in Psychology, 7(566), 1–12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00566.eng
dcterms.referencesBowman, L., Kovelman, L., Hu, X., & Wellman, H. (2015). Children’s belief- and desire-reasoning in the temporoparietal junction: Evidence for specialization from functional near-infrared spectroscopy. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 560–571. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00560.eng
dcterms.referencesBülher, E., Bachmann, C., Goyert, H., Heinzel-Gutenbrunner, M., & Kamp-Becker, I. (2011). Differential diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder by means of inhibitory control and ‘Theory of Mind.’. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41, 1718–1726. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-011-1205-1.eng
dcterms.referencesCaillies, S., Bertot, V., Motte, J., Raynaud, C., & Abely, M. (2014). Social cognition in ADHD: Irony understanding and recursive theory of mind. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 35, 3191–3198. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2014.08.002.eng
dcterms.referencesCalle, D. (2017). Filogenia y desarrollo de funciones ejecutivas. Psicogente, 20, 368–381. https://doi.org/10.17081/psico.20.38.2557.spa
dcterms.referencesCañabate, C. (2014). Cognición social. En R. Redolar (ed). Neurociencia cognitiva, 693–717). Madrid: Editorial Panamericana.spa
dcterms.referencesCardona, M., & Varela, V. (2017). Desempeño lector en niños con diagnóstico de TDAH (Trastorno de Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad). Psicogente, 20, 99–117. https://doi.org/10.17081/psico.20.37.2421.spa
dcterms.referencesCarlson, S. M., & Moses, L. J. (2001). Individual differences in inhibitory control and children’s theory of mind. Child Development, 72(4), 1032–1053. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00333.eng
dcterms.referencesCarlson, S., Moses, L., & Breton, C. (2002). How specific is the relation between executive function and theory of mind? Contributions of inhibitory control and working memory. Infant and Child Development, 11, 73–92. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.298.eng
dcterms.referencesCarlson, S., Moses, L., & Claxton, L. (2004a). Individual differences in executive functioning and theory of mind: An investigation of inhibitory control and planning ability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87, 299–319. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2004.01.002.eng
dcterms.referencesCarlson, S. M., Mandell, D. J., & Williams, L. (2004b). Executive function and theory of mind: Stability and prediction from ages 2 to 3. Developmental Psychology, 40, 1105–1122. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.1105.eng
dcterms.referencesCarlson, S. M., Koenig, M. A., & Harms, M. B. (2013). Theory of mind. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 4, 391–402. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1232.eng
dcterms.referencesCastellanos, F. X., & Tannock, R. (2002). Neuroscience of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: The search for endophenotypes. Nature Review Neuroscience, 3(8), 617–628. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn896.eng
dcterms.referencesCaye, A., Rocha, T. B.-M., Anselmi, L., Murray, J., Menezes, A. M. B., Barros, F. C., & Rohde, L. A. (2016). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder trajectories from childhood to young adulthood evidence from a birth cohort supporting a late-onset syndrome. JAMA Psychiatry, 73, 705–712. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.0383.eng
dcterms.referencesChahín-Pinzón, N. (2015). Consideraciones y reflexiones acerca de la versión colombiana de la escala Barratt de impulsividad para niños (BIS-11c). Psicogente, 18, 396–405. https://doi.org/10.17081/psico.18.34.514.spa
dcterms.referencesCharman, T., Carroll, F., & Sturge, C. (2001). Theory of mind, executive function and social competence in boys with ADHD. Emotional and Behavioural Difficulties, 6, 31–49. https://doi.org/10.1080/13632750100507654.eng
dcterms.referencesCheung, A. M., Mitsis, E. M., & Halperin, J. M. (2004). The relationship of behavioral inhibition to executive functions in young adults. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 26, 393–404. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803390490510103.eng
dcterms.referencesCroca, M., Lagodka, A., Gadel, R., Bourdel, M. C., Bendjemaa, N., Gaillard, R., Olié, J. P., Champagne-Lavau, M., Krebs, M. O., & Amado, I. (2018). Theory of mind and schizophrenia in young and middle-aged patients: Influence of executive functions. Psychiatry Research, 259, 532–537. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2017.10.041.eng
dcterms.referencesCzłapa, K., Wysok, D., & Rybakowski, F. (2016). Deficits in social cognition in autistic spectrum disorders – Comparison with schizophrenia. Neuropsychiatriatry & Neuropsychology (Poland), 11, 12–20. https://doi.org/10.5114/nan.2016.59834.eng
dcterms.referencesDai, X., Gong, Y., Tang, Q., Cai, T., Zhou, S., Cheng, Z., Yao, S., Gio, B., & Xie, Y. (1998). The Chinese intelligence scale for young children: Development strategy, item analysis, reliability, and national norms. Chinese Journal of Clinical Psychology, 6(1), 1–7.eng
dcterms.referencesDe Luca, C., & Leventer, R. (2008). Developmental trajectories of executive functions across the lifespan. In V. Anderson, R. Jacobs, & P. Anderson (Eds.), Executive function~ and the frontal lobes: A lifespan perspective (pp. 23–56). New York: Taylor & Francis Group, LLC.eng
dcterms.referencesErika-Florence, M., Leech, R., & Hampshire, A. (2014). A functional network perspective on response inhibition and attentional control. Nature Communications, 5. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms5073.eng
dcterms.referencesEşsizoğlu, A., Köşger, F., Özlem-Akarsu, F., Özaydin, Ö., & Güleç, G. (2017). Theory of mind and selective attention, response inhibition, cognitive flexibility in patients with schizophrenia. Archives of Neuropsychiatry (Turkey), 54(2), 162–167. https://doi.org/10.5152/npa.2016.12750.eng
dcterms.referencesFahie, C. M., & Symons, D. K. (2003). Executive functioning and theory of mind in children clinically referred for attention and behavior problems. Applied Developmental Psychology, 24, 51–73. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0193-3973(03)00024-8.eng
dcterms.referencesFenollar-Cortés, J., Parra-Martínez, J., Hernández-Pérez, E., & Rabadán-Rubio, J.-A. (2017). Escala HIDEA de cribado escolar orientada al profesorado para detectar marcadores TDAH en primaria. Psicothema, 29, 329–334. https://doi.org/10.7334/psicothema2016.246.eng
dcterms.referencesFlavell, J. (1999). Cognitive development: Children’s knowledge about the mind. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 21–45. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.50.1.21.eng
dcterms.referencesFlores, J., Ostrosky, F., & Lozano, A. (2012). BANFE. Batería Neuropsicológica de funciones ejecutivas y lóbulos frontales. México: Manual Moderno.spa
dcterms.referencesFlynn, E. (2006). A microgenetic investigation of stability and continuity in theory of mind development. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24, 631–654. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151005X57422.eng
dcterms.referencesFlynn, E., O’Malley, C., & Wood, D. (2004). A longitudinal, microgenetic study of the emergence of false belief understanding and inhibition skills. Developmental Science, 7, 103–115. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00326.x.eng
dcterms.referencesFörster, K., Jörgens, S., Air, T. M., Bürger, C., Enneking, V., Redlich, R., Zaremba, D., Grotegerd, D., Dohm, K., Meinert, S., Leehr, E. J., Böhnlein, J., Repple, J., Opel, N., Kavakbasi, E., Arolt, V., Zwitserlood, P., Dannlowski, U., & Baune, B. T. (2018). The relationship between social cognition and executive function in major depressive disorder in high-functioning adolescents and young adults. Psychiatry Research, 263, 139–146. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2018.02.046.eng
dcterms.referencesFuster, J. (2002). Frontal lobe and cognitive development. Journal of Neurocytology, 31(3–5), 373–385. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:102419042.eng
dcterms.referencesGardner, D. M., & Gerdes, A. C. (2015). A review of peer relationships and friendships in youth with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 19, 844–855. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054713501552.eng
dcterms.referencesGeng, J., & Vosel, S. (2013). Re-evaluating the role of TPJ in attentional control: Contextual updating? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 37, 2608–2620. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.08.010.eng
dcterms.referencesGonzález-Gadea, M. L., Baez, S., Torralva, T., Castellanos, F. X., Rattazzi, A., Bein, V., & Ibañez, A. (2013). Cognitive variability in adults with ADHD and AS: Disentangling the roles of executive functions and social cognition. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34, 817–830. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2012.11.009.eng
dcterms.referencesGopnik, A., & Wellman, H. (2002). La teoría de la teoría. In L. Hirschfeld & S. Gelman (Eds.), Cartografía de la mente, la especificidad de dominio en la cognición y en la cultura (pp. 13–63). España: Gedisa Editorial.spa
dcterms.referencesGranta, N., Lawrencea, M., Pretib, A., Wykesa, T., & Cella, M. (2017). Social cognition interventions for people with schizophrenia: A systematic review focusing on methodological quality and intervention modality. Clinical Psychology Review, 56, 55–64. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2017.06.001.eng
dcterms.referencesHenry, J., Philips, L., Ruffman, T., & Bailey, P. (2013). A meta-analytic review of age differences in theory of mind. Psychology and Aging, 28, 826–839. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030677.spa
dcterms.referencesHuang, A. X., Hughes, T. L., Sutton, L. R., Lawrence, M., Chen, X., Ji, Z., & Zeleke, W. (2017). Understanding the self in individuals with autism spectrum disorders (ASD): A review of literature. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1422. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01422.eng
dcterms.referencesHughes, C. (1998). Executive function in preschoolers: Links with theory of mind and verbal ability. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16, 233–253. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.1998.tb00921.x.eng
dcterms.referencesHughes, C., Ensor, R., & Marks, A. (2011). Individual differences in false belief understanding are stable from 3 to 6 years of age and predict children’s mental state talk with school friends. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 96–112. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.07.012.eng
dcterms.referencesHutchins, T., Prelock, P., & Bonazinga, L. (2012). Psychometric evaluation of the theory of mind inventory (TOMI): A study of typically developing children and children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, 327–341. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-011-1244-7.eng
dcterms.referencesJiménez-Figueroa, G., Ardial-Duarte, C., Pineda, D., Acosta-López, J., Cervantes-Henriquez, M., Pineda-Alhucema, W., Cervantes-Gutiérrez, J., Quintero-Ibarra, M., Sánchez-Rojas, M., Vélez, J. I., & Puentes-Rozo, P. (2017). Prepotent response inhibition and reaction times in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder from a Caribbean community. ADHD Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders, 9(4), 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12402-017-0223-z.eng
dcterms.referencesJones, C. R. G., Simonoff, E., Baird, G., Pickles, A., Marsden, A. J. S., Tregay, J., Happe, F., & Charman, T. (2018). The association between theory of mind, executive function, and the symptoms of autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 11, 95–109. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1873.eng
dcterms.referencesJoseph, R. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2004). The relationship of theory of mind and executive functions to symptom type and severity in children with autism. Development and Psychopathology, 16, 137–155. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457940404444X.eng
dcterms.referencesKerlinger, F., & Lee, H. (2002). Investigación del comportamiento. México: McGraw Hill.spa
dcterms.referencesKuijper, S. J. M., Hartman, C.A., & Hendriks, P. (2015). Who is he? Children with ASD and ADHD take the listener into account in their production of ambiguous pronouns. PLoS ONE, 10, art. no. e0132408. Doi: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0132408.eng
dcterms.referencesKuijper, S. J. M., Hartman, C. A., Bogaerds-Hazenberg, S. T. M., & Hendriks, P. (2017). Narrative production in children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Similarities and differences. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 126, 63–75. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000231.eng
dcterms.referencesLangdon, R., Coltheart, M., Ward, P. B., & Catts, S. V. (2001). Mentalising, executive planning and disengagement in schizophrenia. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 6, 81–108. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546800042000061.eng
dcterms.referencesLeslie, A. M. (1987). Pretense and representation: The origins of “theory of mind”. Psychological Review, 94, 412–426. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.94.4.412.eng
dcterms.referencesLezak, M. (1995). Neuropsychological assessment. UK: Oxford University Press.eng
dcterms.referencesLi, X., Hu, D., Deng, W., Tao, Q., Hu, Y., Yang, X., Wang, Z., Tao, R., Yang, L. & Zhang, X. (2017). Pragmatic ability deficit in schizophrenia and associated theory of mind and executive function. Frontiers in Psychology, 8(DEC 11), Article number 2164. Doi: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02164.eng
dcterms.referencesLieberman, M. (2012). A geographical history of social cognitive neuroscience. NeuroImage, 61, 432–436. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.089.eng
dcterms.referencesLogue, S., & Gould, T. (2014). The neural and genetic basis of executive function: Attention, cognitive flexibility, and response inhibition. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 123, 45–54. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pbb.2013.08.007.eng
dcterms.referencesMarín-Méndez, J. J., Borra-Ruiz, M. C., Álvarez-Gómez, M. J., & Soutullo Esperón, C. (2017). Desarrollo psicomotor y dificultades del aprendizaje en preescolares con probable trastorno por déficit de atención e hiperactividad. Estudio epidemiológico en Navarra y La Rioja. Neurologia, 32, 487–493. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nrl.2016.02.009.spa
dcterms.referencesMary, A., Slama, H., Mousty, P., Massat, I., Capiau, T., Drabs, V., & Peigneux, P. (2015). Executive and attentional contributions to theory of mind deficit in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Child Neuropsychology, 22, 345–365. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2015.1012491.eng
dcterms.referencesMcAlister, A., & Peterson, C. (2007). A longitudinal study of child siblings and theory of mind development. Cognitive Development, 22, 258–270. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2006.10.009.eng
dcterms.referencesMesulam, M. M. (2002). The human frontal lobes: Transcending the default mode through contingent encoding. In D. T. Stuss & R. T. Knight (Eds.), Principles of frontal lobe functioning (pp. 8–30). Oxford: Oxford University Press.eng
dcterms.referencesMiranda, A., Berenguer, C., Roselló, B., Baixauli, I., & Colomer, C. (2017). Social cognition in children with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Associations with executive functions. Frontiers in Psychology, 8(1035). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01035.eng
dcterms.referencesMoher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., Altman, D. G., & Group P. (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA statement. BMJ, 339, b2535. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.b2535.eng
dcterms.referencesMoher, D., Shamseer, L., Clarke, M., Ghersi, D., Liberati, A., Petticrew, M., Shekelle, P., & Stewart, L. A. (2015). Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (PRISMA-P) statement. Systematic Reviews, 4(1), 1. https://doi.org/10.1186/2046-4053-4-1.eng
dcterms.referencesMorton, A. (1980). There are many modular theories of mind. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 3, 29–29. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00001655.eng
dcterms.referencesMoses, L., & Tahiroglu, D. (2010). Clarifying the relation between executive function and children’s theories of mind. In B. Sokol, U. Muller, J. Carpendale, A. Young, & G. Iarocci (Eds.), Self- and social-regulation: Exploring the relations between social interaction, social understanding, and the development of executive functions (pp. 218–233). New York: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195327694.003.0009.eng
dcterms.referencesNigg, J. (2006). What causes ADHD? Understanding what goes wrong and why. New York/Lindon: The Guilford Press.eng
dcterms.referencesNoreika, V., Falter, C. M., & Rubia, K. (2013). Timing deficits in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Evidence from neurocognitive and neuroimaging studies. Neuropsychologia, 51(2), 235–266. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.09.036.eng
dcterms.referencesNormand, S., Schneider, B. H., Lee, M. D., Masionneuve, M. F., Keuhn, S. M., & Robaey, P. (2011). How do children with ADHD (mis)manage their real-life dyadic friendships? A multi-method investigation. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39, 293–305. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-010-9450-x.eng
dcterms.referencesNydén, A., Niklasson, L., Stahlberg, O., Anckarsater, H., Wentz, E., Rastam, M., & Gillberg, C. (2010). Adults with autism spectrum disorders and ADHD neuropsychological aspects. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 31, 1659–1668. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2010.04.010.eng
dcterms.referencesOsterhaus, C., Koerber, S., & Sodian, B. (2016). Scaling of advanced theory-of-mind tasks. Child Development, 87, 1971–1991. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12566.eng
dcterms.referencesOzonoff, S., & McEvoy, R. E. (1994). A longitudinal study of executive function and theory of mind development in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 6(03), 415–431. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579400006027.eng
dcterms.referencesOzonoff, S., Penningtion, B., & Rogers, S. (1991). Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: Relationship to theory of mind. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 1081–1105. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1991.tb00351.x.eng
dcterms.referencesPascual, B., Aguado, G., & Sotillo, M. (2006). Aproximación a las diferentes perspectivas teóricas sobre la «teoría de la mente. Revista De Logopedia, Foniatria y Audiologia, 26, 154–165. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0214-4603(06)70208-X.spa
dcterms.referencesPavarinni, G., & De Hollanda, D. (2010). Teoria da mente, empatia e motivaçãopró-social emcriançaspré-escolares. PsicologiaemEstudo, 15, 613–622.eng
dcterms.referencesPerner, J., Lang, B., & Kloo, D. (2002). Theory of mind and self-control: More than a common problem of inhibition. Child Development, 73, 752–767. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00436.eng
dcterms.referencesPeterson, B. S., Potenza, M. N., Wang, Z., Zhu, H., Martin, A., Marsh, R., Plessen, K. J., & Yu, S. (2009). An FMRI study of the effects of psychostimulants on default-mode processing during Stroop task performance in youths with ADHD. American Journal of Psychiatry, 166(11), 1286–1294. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.08050724.eng
dcterms.referencesPiazzarollo, C., & De Hollanda, D. (2013). A Relação entre Teoria da Mente e Desenvolvimento Moral emCriançasPré-Escolares. Psideia, 23, 93–101. https://doi.org/10.1590/1982-43272354201311.por
dcterms.referencesPickup, G. (2008). Relationship between theory of mind and executive function in schizophrenia: A systematic review. Psychopathology, 41, 206–213. https://doi.org/10.1159/000125554.eng
dcterms.referencesPineda-Alhucema, W. (2017). Sobre la valoración de la Teoría de la Mente. Psicogente, 20, 1–2. https://doi.org/10.17081/psico.20.38.2571.spa
dcterms.referencesPineda-Alhucema, W., Jiménez, G., & Puentes, P. (2012). Retrospectiva y prospectiva de la teoría de la mente; Avances de investigación en neurociencias. Psicogente, 15, 178–197.spa
dcterms.referencesPineda-Alhucema, W., Escudero-Cabarcas, J., & Vásquez, F. (2015). Cognición social en el Trastorno por Déficit de Atención-Hiperactividad: El lenguaje pragmático como indicador de teoría de la mente en niños/as con TDAH. In Y. Alarcón, F. Vásquez, W. Pineda-Alhucema, & Y. Martínez (Eds.), Estudios actuales en psicología (pp. 19–32). Barranquilla: Ediciones Universidad Simón Bolívar.spa
dcterms.referencesPitzianti, M., Grelloni, C., Casarelli, L., D’Agati, E., Spiridigliozzi, S., Curatolo, P., & Pasini, A. (2017). Neurological soft signs, but not theory of mind and emotion recognition deficit distinguished children with ADHD from healthy control. Psychiatry Research, 256, 96–101. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2017.06.029.eng
dcterms.referencesPremack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 515–528. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00076512.eng
dcterms.referencesPuentes, P., Jiménez, G., Pineda-Alhucema, W., Pimienta, D., Acosta, J., Cervantes, M., & Sánchez, M. (2014). Déficit en Habilidades Sociales en Niños con Trastorno por Déficit de Atención-Hiperactividad, Evaluados con la Escala BASC. Revista Colombiana de Psicología, 23, 95–106. https://doi.org/10.15446/rcp.v23n1.34332.spa
dcterms.referencesPujals, E., Batlle, S., Camprodon, E., Pujals, S., Estrada, X., Aceña, M., … Pérez-Solá, V. (2016). Brief report: Translation and adaptation of the theory of mind inventory to Spanish. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46, 685–690. Doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2576-5.eng
dcterms.referencesReboul, A. (2006). Theory of mind or simulation: The contribution of developmental studies [Théorie de l’espritou simulation: l’Apport des étudesdéveloppementales]. Confrontations Psychiatriques, 46, 117–126.spa
dcterms.referencesRoazzi, A., & De Melo, S. (1999). Teoria da Mente: efeito da idade, do sexo e do uso de atores animados e inanimados na inferência de estados mentais. Psicología Reflexao e Crítica, 12(2), 307–330. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79721999000200005.spa
dcterms.referencesRubia, K., Halari, R., Cubillo, A., Mohammad, A.-M., Brammer, M., & Taylor, E. (2009). Methylphenidate normalises activation and functional connectivity deficits in attention and motivation networks in medication-naive children with ADHD during a rewarded continuous performance task. Neuropharmacology, 57(7–8), 640–652. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropharm.2009.08.013.eng
dcterms.referencesRusso, D., Arteaga, F., Rubiales, J., & Bakker, L. (2015). Competencia social y status sociométrico escolar en niños y niñas cn TDAH. Revista Latinoamericana de Ciencias Sociales, Niñez y Juventud, 13, 1081–1091. https://doi.org/10.11600/1692715x.13237190514.spa
dcterms.referencesSabbagh, M. A., Xu, F., Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Lee, K. (2006). The development of executive functioning and theory of mind: A comparison of Chinese and U.S. preschoolers. Psychological Science, 17(1), 74–81. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01667.x.eng
dcterms.referencesSaxe, R. (2006). Four brain regions for one theory of mind. En J. Cacioppo, P. Visser, & C. Pickett. Social neuroscience. Massachusetts: MIT press.eng
dcterms.referencesSaxe, R., & Powell, L. J. (2006). It’s the thought that counts: Specific brain regions for one component of theory of mind. Psychological Science, 17, 692–699. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01768.x.eng
dcterms.referencesSaxe, R., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., Scholz, J., & Pelphrey, K. (2009). Brain regions for perceiving and reasoning about other people in school-aged children. Child Development, 80, 1197–1209. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01325.x.eng
dcterms.referencesScholl, B., & Leslie, A. (1999). Modularity, development and ‘theory of mind’. Mind and Language, 14, 131–153. https://doi.org/10.1111/1468-0017.00106.eng
dcterms.referencesShuai, L., Chan, R. C., & Wang, Y. (2011). Executive function profile of Chinese boys with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: Different subtypes and comorbidity. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 26, 120–132. https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acq101.eng
dcterms.referencesSodian, B., & Hülsken, C. (2005). The developmental relation of theory of mind and executive functions: A study of advanced theory of mind abilities in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. In W. Schneider, R. Schumann-Hengsteler, & B. Sodian (Eds.), Young children’s cognitive development: Interrelationships among executive functioning, working memory, verbal ability, and theory of mind (pp. 175–188). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.eng
dcterms.referencesStuss, D. T. (2011). Functions of the frontal lobes: Relation to executive functions. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17, 759–765. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617711000695.eng
dcterms.referencesTakeuchi, H., Taki, Y., Sassa, Y., Hashizume, H., Sekiguchi, A., Fukushima, A., & Kawashima, R. (2013). Brain structures associated with executive functions during everyday events in a non-clinical simple. Brain Structure and Function, 2018(4), 1017–1032. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-012-0444-z.eng
dcterms.referencesVan-Stralen, J. (2016). Emotional dysregulation in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. ADHD Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders, 8, 175–187. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12402-016-0199-0.eng
dcterms.referencesVaquerizo-Madrid, J., & Estevez-Diaz, L. (2006). Revisión del modelo de alerta e intervención psicoloinguística en el trastorno de déficit de atención por hiperactividad. Revolucion Neurologica, Suplemento, 2, 53–61.spa
dcterms.referencesWellman, H. M. (2014). Making minds. UK: Oxford University Press.eng
dcterms.referencesWellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2004). Scaling of theory of mind tasks. Child Development, 75, 523–541. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00691.x.eng
dcterms.referencesWellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72, 655–684. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00304.eng
dcterms.referencesYanéz-Téllez, G., Romero-Romero, H., Rivera-Garcia, L., Prieto-Corona, B., Bernal Hernandez, J., Marosi-Holczberger, E., Guerrero-Juarez, V., Rodríguez-Camacho, M., & Silva-Pereyra, J. F. (2012). Funciones cognoscitivas y ejecutivas en el TDAH. Actas Españolas de Psiquiatría, 40(6), 293–298.spa
dcterms.referencesYang, J., Zhou, S., Yao, S., Su, L., & McWhinnie, C. (2009). The relationship between theory of mind and executive function in a sample of children from mainland China. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 40, 169–182. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-008-0119-4.eng
dcterms.referencesYang, D., Rosenblau, G., Keifer, C., & Pelphrey, K. (2015). An integrative model of social perception, action observation and theory of mind. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 51, 263–275. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.01.020.eng
dcterms.referencesYee-Mikami, A., Jia, M., & Jiwon, J. (2014). Social skills training. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 4, 775–788. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chc.2014.05.007.eng
dcterms.referencesZelazo, P. D., & Müller, U. (2011). Executive function in typical and atypical development. In U. Goswami (Ed.), The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development (2nd ed., pp. 574–603). Malden: Wiley-Blackwell.eng
dcterms.referencesZuluaga, J., Marín, L., & Becerra, A. (2018). Teoría de la mente y empatía en niños y niñas con diagnóstico de síndrome de asperger y un grupo control. Psicogente, 21(39), 88–101. https://doi.org/10.17081/psico.21.39.2824.spa

Archivos

Bloque de licencias
Mostrando 1 - 1 de 1
No hay miniatura disponible
Nombre:
license.txt
Tamaño:
368 B
Formato:
Item-specific license agreed upon to submission
Descripción:

Colecciones